About the Author(s)


Thokozile I. Metsing Email symbol
Department of Optometry, Faculty of Health Sciences, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa

Citation


Metsing TI. The effect of induced blur monocularly and binocularly on stereoacuity. Afr Vision Eye Health. 2023;82(1), a846. https://doi.org/10.4102/aveh.v82i1.846

Original Research

The effect of induced blur monocularly and binocularly on stereoacuity

Thokozile I. Metsing

Received: 24 Feb. 2023; Accepted: 29 June 2023; Published: 23 Oct. 2023

Copyright: © 2023. The Author(s). Licensee: AOSIS.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Abstract

Background: Stereopsis plays a significant role in our visual system because it allows perception of depth to perform day-to-day activities.

Aim: The aim of this study was to determine the severity and proportions of monocular versus binocular-induced blur on stereoacuity using the Randot® Stereo Test (RST) at near (40 cm).

Setting: Data were collected at the optometry clinic of the University of Johannesburg.

Methods: The study used a cross-sectional, experimental and prospective design with a quantitative approach. Forty university students between the ages of 18 years and 25 years were invited to participate in this study. Blur was induced monocularly and binocularly through 1 dioptre (D) to 3 D lenses and stereoacuity was measured using the RST at near (40 cm).

Results: Stereoacuity measured with the added 1 D to 3 D lenses monocularly and binocularly resulted in reduced stereopsis, which was worse monocularly, found not to be significant (p = 0.17) with the addition of the 1 D lens(es) monocularly and binocularly. Significant reductions in stereoacuity were found with the addition of 2 D (p = 0.00) and 3 D (p = 0.00) monocularly and binocularly, respectively.

Conclusion: The effect of induced optical blur monocularly on stereoacuity impacted significantly compared with induced binocular blur. However, the different accommodative amplitudes could have affected monocularly and binocularly induced stereoacuity for some participants.

Contribution: The study is clinical and focuses on the effect of induced blur monocularly and binocularly on stereoacuity. Key insights are that the effect of induced optical blur monocularly impacted significantly on stereoacuity compared with induced blur binocularly.

Keywords: stereoacuity; isometropia; anisometropia; depth perception; induced blur; retinal disparity; optical blur.

Introduction

Stereopsis is a perceptual phenomenon at the centre of scientific observations after the stereoscope was invented in 1838 by Wheatstone.1 Our brain uses two-dimensional images from both the left and right eyes, to enable the description of three-dimensional structures. Therefore, this process makes it possible to detect depth as a visual experience using the technique of stereopsis.2 Differential focusing, image overlap, motion parallax, perspective and shading are some of the monocular cues to depth, while the vergence position of the eyes and binocular disparity are the two exclusive binocular cues to depth. Stereopsis enhances the ability to discriminate depth differences.3,4

The ophthalmic and binocular visual integration systems rely heavily on stereoacuity, viewed as the resolving capacity of the visual system.5 Stereoacuity enables the matching of reality and perception, required for survival and learning in our environments. Binocular vision not only provides binocular depth perception but it also improves performance on tasks such as discrimination, resolution and detection.4,5 Besides the above-mentioned fundamental tasks, binocular vision enables effective performance of complex visual tasks such as reading, eye–hand coordination and detecting camouflaged objects even in the absence of depth.5,6 O’Connor discovered that subjects with normal stereoacuity performed better on motor skills tasks than subjects with poor stereoacuity. The study also demonstrated that normal stereoacuity is required for everyday tasks such as pouring water into a jug, inserting a needle into a bead and reading.6

One of the cues people utilise to perceive depth is retinal disparity, which is determined by the binocular depth perception, or stereopsis. There are two types of stereopsis: local and global, which vary in the amount of retinal area covered.7 In this study, the random dot test (RDT) will be used to assess overall stereoacuities. The term ‘binocular disparity’ describes the variation in image locations and forms between two eyes, induced by the vantage points through which the eyes view the world.8 Stereoacuity, contrast sensitivity and visual acuity (VA) are only a few of the many visual characteristics that constitute vision.9 Our lives are growing more dependent on stereoacuity and binocularity because of the demand for 3-D awareness of our motor skills ability.10 Most of the images we see are best understood through binocularity and stereoacuity. In addition, stereoacuity is necessary for depth perception to carry out many daily tasks and correctly perceive moving objects.11

Blur, amblyopia or strabismus interfere with stereoacuity. Anisometropia is one of the prominent visual anomalies causing amblyopia and strabismus in children. The loss of binocular function in people with anisometropia is universally acknowledged.10,11 Binocularity is anticipated to be more negatively impacted by higher degrees of anisometropia than by lower degrees of anisometropia.12 Research conducted supports that an uncorrected anisometropic refractive error (RE) can lead to foveal suppression and result in reduced binocular depth perception.13,14 Furthermore, after producing anisometropia or aniseikonia in adults with healthy binocular vision, experimental studies have documented a significant decrease in stereopsis, although the degree of stereopsis and the severity of anisometropia was found to not significantly correlate.15

In a study by Oguz and Oguz13 on 21 healthy adults ranging in age from 22 years to 34 years, they induced unilateral anisometropia for different types of REs, namely myopia, hyperopia or astigmatism (90/45 degrees). In their study, it was found that a dioptre of induced spherical anisometropia reduced stereoacuity by an average of 57–59 arcseconds (ʺ), while induced cylindrical anisometropia reduced stereoacuity by 51ʺ – 56ʺ.12 The findings of the study conducted by Dadeya et al.,14 which investigated the effect of induced spherical and cylindrical anisometropia, also showed a significant reduction of stereoacuity. Furthermore, the effect of induced blur using the cylindrical and spherical lenses (i.e., positive or negative) was found to lead to the deterioration of stereoacuity, with hypermetropic astigmatism leading to more adverse effects than myopic astigmatism.13

According to a study conducted by Hoosen et al.15 induced binocular blur was found to have reduced proportional to the power of the convex test lens. They concluded that monocularly reduced vision should be considered when screening uncorrected and corrected emmetropes for eligibility of a driver’s license. However, a VA of 6/9 or better in at least one eye is the requirement for drivers to qualify for vehicle licenses in South Africa.16 The determination of the proportional decrease of induced stereoacuity monocularly and binocularly is likely to provide information related to its severity, which could contribute to the reduction of motor vehicle accidents. However, there are still ongoing debates related to reduced vision monocularly or binocularly and their severity on stereoacuity.17

Wajuihian and Hasraj,18,19 in their study investigated the relationship between stereoacuity, refractive status, accommodative and vergence anomalies of South African school children with a mean age 15.89 ± 1.58 years and concluded that accommodative and convergence dysfunctions affected stereoacuity. Furthermore, participants with anisometropia were found to have significantly reduced mean stereoacuities compared to those with emmetropia. Therefore, they concluded that REs, accommodative or vergence anomalies are likely to contribute to the reduction of mean stereoacuity. As a result, they suggested that the Randot® Stereo Test (RST) could be used to identify those with REs, accommodative or vergence anomalies and further supported stereoacuity measurements as a useful tool to screen for binocular anomalies.18,19 However, the aim of this study was to determine the severity and amount of stereoacuity of induced blur monocularly and binocularly using the RST at near (40 cm).

Methods

The research study was based on cross-sectional and experimental designs with a quantitative approach. A total of 40 participants were invited from university students, aged between 18 years and 26 years, with males (n = 30) and females (n = 10) from different ethnic groups. Recruitment of participants was carried out through social media platforms such as Facebook, Instagram and by word of mouth. The collection of data adhered to the ethics requirements of privacy, confidentiality and anonymity. Code names were allocated to participants to maintain anonymity, the data were collected in an eye testing cubicle by the researcher on the participant, in the absence of any other person and confidentiality was maintained by saving the collected data in a password-coded document only accessible to the researcher and supervisor. Before the commencement of data collection, purposively selected participants were handed the information and consent letter to sign. After signing the consent forms, their visual status was screened to ensure they met the inclusion criteria.

The inclusion criteria for participating in the study was VA of 6/6 or better monocularly and binocularly with or without habitual RE corrected. Participants with REs between ±1 dioptre (D) were invited to participate in the study. Excluded from the study were those with a history of strabismus, ocular pathologies, REs worse than ±1 D and minors. Tests used to screen for eligibility of participants were a comprehensive case history, determination of REs objectively using the autorefractor, cover test (CT) and investigation of ocular health status using the slit lamp and a fundus camera to rule out ocular pathologies. Eligible participants were allocated code names for the purpose of anonymity. Confidentiality was upheld by storing all collected data in a password-encrypted document only accessible to the researcher. Four separate stations were used to collect data. At the first station, the demographic data of participants were collected with one research assistant conducting vision screening to check the VA, and autorefractor and CT were conducted. Baseline stereo acuity was measured (through habitual prescription) and with 1 D-induced monocular and binocular stereoacuities. Three measurements for the baseline and 1 D, 2 D and 3 D-induced monocular and binocular stereoacuities were taken with the average for each category recorded. The final evaluation was of induced monocular and binocular stereoacuities using 2 D and 3 D lenses measured separately. Loose lenses were used with a trial frame to induce optical blur. Stereoacuity was measured at 40 cm as research shows no stereoacuity differences at a distance (5.18 m) and near (40 cm) under normal viewing conditions.20 A pair of 3 D polarising spectacles were used to allow minimisation of monocular cues.

The RST used had targets with disparities of 400ʺ, 200ʺ, 160ʺ, 100ʺ, 63ʺ, 50ʺ, 40ʺ, 32ʺ, 25ʺ and 20ʺ with the highest values representing reduced stereoacuities and lower values demonstrating good stereoacuities. For participants unable to appreciate the difference at maximum disparity 400ʺ, their stereoacuity was recorded as null and they were given an arbitrary value of 600ʺ for statistical purposes.9 Consistency was upheld by data collectors with the RST chart held parallel and perpendicular to the participants’ line of sight to eliminate monocular cues. No squinting or movements of the stereoacuity booklet for a better view of targets was allowed. The illumination in the testing rooms was not measured but it was kept at the same level by using the same fluorescent lamp throughout the process of data collection to maintain accuracy. Furthermore, the test cubicles where data were collected were completely insulated from ambient light. Participants were instructed to identify stereoscopic circles, and the identified maximum stereo circle was recorded as their stereoacuity.

Data analysis

The results of this study were evaluated for similarities and differences to check for normality of data distribution, medians, means, kurtosis and skewness including standard deviations of measured stereoacuities. Quantitative data were collected and analysed using Microsoft Excel. Data analysis also included graphical methods of box and whisker plots. Spearman’s correlation test was used to test the association between stereo acuities of monocularly versus binocularly induced blur compared with baseline stereopsis. Inferential statistics such as t-tests and r-values were determined to clarify the significance and strength of differences in stereoacuity outcomes compared with baseline, induced blur monocularly and binocularly using the lenses. The criteria used for ranking of results using the RST at near are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1: Ranking of results for stereoacuity testing in seconds of arc relevant to a paediatric population.
Ethical considerations

Ethical clearance to conduct this study was obtained from the University of Johannesburg, Faculty of Health Sciences Research Ethics Committee (reference no.: REC-1406-2022).

Results

Forty participants with ages ranging from 18 years to 25 years had interpupillary distances (IPD) measured, near point of convergence (NPC), VA, CT and objective REs using the autorefractor for screening purposes. Vision screening tests were used to ensure that those selected to participate in the study had no binocular vision anomalies (strabismus and heterophoria), high REs and poor vision both at distance and near. The collected data were not analysed in terms of gender, age and/or race as the sample was relatively small. The focus of the study was to investigate the effect of induced blur with 1 D, 2 D and 3 D lenses, monocularly (simulating anisometropia) and binocularly (simulating isometropia) on stereoacuity compared with the baseline.

Descriptive statistics for all variables

Table 2 shows descriptive statistics of analysed stereoacuities data on baseline and induced blur monocularly and binocularly using 1 D, 2 D and 3 D lenses. Baseline near stereoacuity data collected were spread between 20ʺ and 30ʺ with the mode and median of 20ʺ. Therefore, showing that all participants had stereoacuity that fell within the normal ranges. However, the data collected showed high positive skewness of 1.14, with majority of participants found to have 20ʺ and a mean of 23 ± 4ʺ. The minimum and maximum stereoacuity values were 20ʺ and 30ʺ, respectively (Table 2). The standard deviation of the baseline stereoacuity was found to be 4ʺ, indicating that the collected data were clustered around the mean.

TABLE 2: Descriptive statistics for baseline stereoacuity versus induced blur monocularly and binocularly through 1 D, 2 D and 3 D lenses on participants (N = 40).

The means for monocularly induced blur stereoacuities for 1 D, 2 D and 3 D lenses were found to be 67 ± 57ʺ, 192 ± 164ʺ and 410.6 ± 202ʺ, respectively. The mean of binocularly induced blur stereoacuities was found to be 57 ± 28ʺ, 85.8 ± 70ʺ and 200 ± 155ʺ, respectively, compared with those for induced blur monocularly. The standard deviation for stereoacuities increased with induced blur monocularly compared with binocularly. Thus, showing a large amount of variation of measured stereoacuities from the mean as the strength of convex lenses was increased. However, the distribution of data for stereoacuities measured through induced blur monocularly and binocularly was found to be positively skewed. The skewness for stereoacuities measured through 2 D lenses monocularly and binocularly were found to be high > 1. A positive kurtosis of 10.1 showing a leptokurtic distribution, indicated more extreme values than the expected normal probability through induced blur using 2 D lenses binocularly. However, fewer outliers were observed with a low kurtosis of 0.39 for monocular-induced stereoacuities through a 3 D lens.

Baseline stereoacuities compared with induced blur monocularly and binocular using 1 dioptre lenses

All participants were found to have very strong baseline stereoacuity ≤ 30ʺ (Figure 1). However, 12.5% (n = 5) participants demonstrated outliers of 160ʺ for stereoacuities with 1 D lens inducing blur monocularly. The outliers for binocularly induced blur with 1 D lenses were found to be 2.5% (n = 1) also indicating that one participant had weak stereoacuity (Figure 1). Stereoacuity data measured with added 1 D monocularly and binocularly showed positive skewness as demonstrated in Table 2. It is further demonstrated by the shorter whisker on the lower end of the box, confirming probability distribution deviating from symmetrical normal distribution of data collected (Figure 1). The addition of 1 D monocularly, demonstrated radically reduced stereoacuity compared with binocularly induced blur. However, the p-value was found to be > 0.05 (p = 0.17).

FIGURE 1: Baseline stereoacuities compared with induced blur monocularly and binocularly using 1 dioptre lenses.

Baseline stereoacuities compared with induced blur monocularly and binocularly using 2 dioptre lenses

Stereoacuity levels were found to be reduced in proportion to the degree of induced blur monocularly and binocularly in all participants with added 2 D lenses (Figure 2). However, a majority (57.5%) were found to have good stereo acuities (≤ 50ʺ), with 42.5% reporting adequate stereoacuities (50ʺ to 100ʺ) for monocularly induced blur. Fewer participants (32.5%) were found to have good stereoacuities (≤ 50ʺ) for binocularly induced blur, with 20% found to have adequate stereoacuities (50ʺ to 100ʺ) and 17% with weak stereoacuities (101ʺ to 600ʺ), thus the leptokurtic findings of 10.1, for stereoacuities measured binocularly through 2 D lenses. Furthermore, stereoacuity scores for 2 D lenses inducing blur monocularly were found to be very far from the mean compared with binocularly induced blur (192 ± 164 vs 85.8 ± 70), respectively (Table 1). The finding indicates a very high amount of variability worse with 2 D added monocularly than binocularly. Figure 2 shows positive skewness with the shorter whisker on the lower end of the box for monocularly induced blur stereoacuities compared to those induced binocularly. Thus, showing a probability distribution deviating from the symmetrical normal distribution of data collected. Nonetheless, the stereoacuities of monocularly versus binocularly induced blur with 2 D were found to be significant with the p > 0.05 (p = 0.00). However, the r-value indicated a weak correlation (r = 0.45) between stereoacuities measured with blur induced monocularly and binocularly with 2 D lenses.

FIGURE 2: Baseline stereoacuities compared with induced blur monocularly and binocularly using 2 dioptre lenses.

Baseline stereoacuities compared with induced blur monocularly and binocular using 3 dioptre lenses

Reduced stereoacuity with an average of 200 ± 95ʺ with 3 D lenses inducing monocular blur was found (Table 1). Adequate stereoacuities (51ʺ – 100ʺ) were found in 7.5% and a majority (92.5%) had very weak stereoacuities (101ʺ – 600ʺ). Compared with induced blur binocularly, 5% were found to have very strong stereoacuities (≤ 30ʺ), 12.5% had adequate stereoacuities (51ʺ – 100ʺ) and 82.5% were found to have weak to very weak > 100ʺ to 600ʺ (Figure 3). Therefore, this demonstrates that the addition of 3 D monocularly and binocularly reduced stereoacuity significantly (p = 0.00). However, the correlation between monocularly and binocularly induced stereoacuities was found to be weak with r = 0.22. Interestingly, 10% of the study participants had stereoacuities similar to their baseline despite the inducement of binocular blur with 3 D lenses. However, the very same participants to the contrary showed decreased stereopsis of 600ʺ monocularly.

FIGURE 3: Baseline stereoacuities compared with induced blur monocularly and binocularly using 3 dioptre lenses.

Discussion

The findings of this study were found to be consistent with those of other studies that measured the effect of reduced vision monocularly (simulating anisometropia) on stereoacuity and concluded that the reduction of stereopsis increased proportionately to the increased power of plus lenses.12,13,14,15,16,17 Even though the other studies measured stereoacuity using local targets (RST), the final findings are consistent with those of this study. However, in this study, the focus was not only on induced blur monocularly (simulating anisometropia) but also on comparing it with binocularly induced blur (simulating isometropia).

The results of this study were further found to be consistent with those of the study conducted by Yang et al.,21 who found complete loss of stereopsis in patients with spherical hypermetropia > 3 D. Although their study participants had different types of REs including astigmatism, myopia and hyperopia. Contrary to the findings of this study in which myopic blur shift was induced using 1 D, 2 D and 3 D lenses, and other REs such as astigmatism including hyperopic blur shift were not considered.

In this study, inducing blur with 1 D lens resulted in a significant reduction in stereoacuity. However, the association between the induced blur monocularly and binocularly with 1 D and baseline stereoacuity was found to be not significant (p = 0.17). Contrary to the findings of the study conducted by Brooks et al.,22 in which inducing of blur using 1 D lenses was found to have caused a significant reduction in stereoacuity. However, this could be attributed to the age of their participants between 26 years and 59 years and a TNO Stereo Test (TST) was used to measure the stereoacuities. Poor amplitudes of accommodation could have contributed to their study findings because accommodation dysfunctions were also found to impact on stereoacuities.19,20

Even though in this study significant associations (p = 0.00) between induced blur monocularly and binocularly using the 2 D and 3 D lenses were found, their correlation was found to be weak with r = 0.45 and 0.22, respectively. Contrary to the findings of the study conducted by Yang et al.,21 which found a significant correlation between different groups of spherical errors and stereoacuity measured and thus concluded that high hyperopia (> 3 D) severely reduced stereoacuity. Nevertheless, Yang et al.’s21 study was conducted on participants with real REs that were not induced compared with this study.

The findings of a study conducted by Singh et al.,23 which investigated the effects of induced monocular blur with 3 D on surgical tasks in a simulated environment were found to be consistent with those of our study. Therefore, they concluded that induced blur monocularly was associated with a significant reduction of stereopsis in surgical task scores. A significant positive correlation between the level of stereoacuity, measured using Stereo Test (TNO) and Randot® Stereo Test (RTS) versus the surgical task scores was found. Consistent with the findings of our study, which found a weak correlation of r = 0.51 when 1 D was used to induce blur monocularly and binocularly, even though it was found to be significant with 2 D and 3 D lenses. However, the differences between our study and that of Singh et al.23 could be that our study was not conducted in a simulated environment.

In this study, addition of higher plus lenses (1 D, 2 D and 3 D) monocularly resulted in significantly reduced mean stereoacuity compared with when added binocularly. This led to the conclusion that there is some clinical significance between the two stereoacuity measurements with plus lenses added monocularly and binocularly.

Limitations of the study

One limitation of this study is that the screening tests did not include the evaluation of the accommodative amplitudes. Some participants could have had low amplitudes compared with the others, thus the discrepancy in the measurements of stereoacuities because they were only measured at near. The screening tests for visual anomalies only considered the autorefractor results and subjective refraction determining the true status of the REs could give a true reflection of our findings.

Conclusion

The effect of induced optical blur monocularly impacted to a greater extent on stereoacuity compared with binocular-induced blur.

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank K.B. Malate, E. Nyarugwe, R.N. Mphahlele, J.K. Namukwambi, S.S. Maphalala and L.V. Mabu in assisting with the collection of data for this research project.

Competing interests

The author declares that she has no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced her in writing this article.

Author’s contributions

T.I.M. contributed towards the conceptualisation, methodology, write-up, and analysis of the collected data for the research article.

Funding information

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author, T.I.M.

Disclaimer

The views and opinions expressed in this article are those of the author and do not reflect the official policy or position any affiliated agency of the author.

References

  1. Vishwanath D. Toward a new theory of stereopsis. Psychol Rev. 2014;121(2):151. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035233
  2. Poggio GF, Poggio T. The analysis of stereopsis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1984. 7379–7412. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.07.030184.002115
  3. Banks MS, Read JC, Allison RS, Watt SJ. Stereoscopy and the human visual system. SMPTE Motion Imaging J. 2012;121(4):24–43. https://doi.org/10.5594/j18173
  4. Steinman SB, Steinman BA, Garzia RP. Foundations of binocular vision: A clinical perspective. New York: McGraw-Hill Education/Medical; 2000.
  5. Parker AJ. Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(5):379–391. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2131
  6. O’connor AR, Tidbury LP. Stereopsis: Are we assessing it in enough depth? Clin Exp Optom. 2018;101(4):485–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/cxo.12655
  7. Surdick RT, Davis ET, King RA, Hodges LF. The perception of distance in simulated visual displays: A comparison of the effectiveness and accuracy of multiple depth cues across viewing distances. Presence. 1997;6(5):513–531. https://doi.org/10.1162/pres.1997.6.5.513
  8. Umeda K, Tanabe S, Fujita I. Representation of stereoscopic depth based on relative disparity in macaque area V4. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98(1):241–252. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01336.2006
  9. Lovell PG, Bloj M, Harris JM. Optimal integration of shading and binocular disparity for depth perception. J Vis. 2012;12(1):1, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1167/12.1.1
  10. Singh D, Aggarwal S, Sachdeva MM, Saxena R. Effect of induced monocular blur on monocular and binocular visual functions. IJCEO. 2015;1(4):197–201. https://doi.org/10.5958/2395-1451.2015.00015.3
  11. Tarczy-Hornoch K, Varma R, Cotter SA, et al. Risk factors for decreased visual acuity in preschool children: The multiethnic pediatric eye disease and Baltimore pediatric eye. Ophthalmology. 2011;118(11):2262–2273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ophtha.2011.06.033
  12. Oguz H, Oguz V. The effects of experimentally induced anisometropia on stereopsis. JAAPOS. 2000;37(4):214–218. https://doi.org/10.3928/0191-3913-20000701-08
  13. Lee JY, Seo JY, Baek SU. The effects of glasses for anisometropia on stereopsis. Am J Ophthalmol. 2013;156(6):1261–1266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajo.2013.07.016
  14. Dadeya S, Shibal F. The effect of anisometropia on binocular visual function. Indian J Ophthalmol. 2001;49(4):261.
  15. Kulkarni V, Puthran N, Gagal B. Correlation between stereoacuity and experimentally induced graded monocular and binocular astigmatism. JCDR. 2016;10(5):NC14. https://doi.org/10.7860/JCDR/2016/17341.7873
  16. Hoosen T, Mitha F, Narothan C, et al. Effect of induced blur on distance visual function. Afr Vision Eye Health. 2020;79(1):8. https://doi.org/10.4102/aveh.v79i1.575
  17. South Africa Regulation 102 (Replacing Regulation 241) [homepage on the Internet]. [cited 2023 May 18]. Available from: https://www.oasa.org.za/Regulation102.php
  18. Wajuihian SO, Hansraj R. Vergence anomalies in a sample of high school students in South Africa. J Optom. 2016;9(4):246–257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.optom.2015.10.006
  19. Wajuihian SO, Hansraj R. Stereoacuity and refractive, accommodative and vergence anomalies of South African school children, aged 13–18 years. Afr Vision Eye Health. 2018. 26;77(1):1–8. https://doi.org/10.4102/aveh.v77i1.400
  20. Griffin JR, Grisham JD. Binocular anomalies: Diagnosis and vision therapy. Boston: Butterworth Heinemann Medical; 2002.
  21. Yang J, Huang T, Yang K, et al.The effects of hyperopic and astigmatic ametropia on stereoacuity by Titmus stereo test. Taiwan J Ophthalmol. 2012;2(1):22–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tjo.2011.11.004
  22. Brooks SE, Johnson D, Fischer N. Anisometropia and binocularity. Ophthalmology. 1996;103(7):1139–1143. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0161-6420(96)30555-1
  23. Singh P, Bergaal SK, Sharma P, Agarwal T, Saxena R, Phuljhele S. Effect of induced anisometropia on stereopsis and surgical tasks in a simulated environment. Indian J Ophthalmol. 2021;69(3):568. https://doi.org/10.4103/ijo.IJO_1540_20


Crossref Citations

No related citations found.